can someone write 2 summaries for the 2 articles attached? the summaries need to be no more than 250 words. summaries should include the background, hypotheses, studies, results, and a brief discussi
Stuck with a difficult assignment? No time to get your paper done? Feeling confused? If you’re looking for reliable and timely help for assignments, you’ve come to the right place. We promise 100% original, plagiarism-free papers custom-written for you. Yes, we write every assignment from scratch and it’s solely custom-made for you.
Order a Similar Paper Order a Different Paper
can someone write 2 summaries for the 2 articles attached?
the summaries need to be no more than 250 words. summaries should include the background, hypotheses, studies, results, and a brief discussion.
can someone write 2 summaries for the 2 articles attached? the summaries need to be no more than 250 words. summaries should include the background, hypotheses, studies, results, and a brief discussi
Addressing Neuroticism in Psychological Treatment Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Julianne G. Wilner, and David H. Barlow Boston University Neuroticism has long been associated with psychopathology and there is increasing evidence that this trait represents a shared vulnerability responsible for the development and maintenance of a range of common mental disorders. Given that neuroticism may be more malleable than previously thought, targeting this trait in treatment, rather than its specific manifestations (e.g., anxiety, mood, and person- ality disorders), may represent a more efficient and cost-effective approach to psychological treatment. The goals of the current manuscript are to (a) review the role of neuroticism in the development of common mental disorders, (b) describe the evidence of its malleability, and (c) review interventions that have been explicitly developed to target this trait in treatment. Implications for shifting the focus of psychological treatment to underlying vulnerabilities, such as neuroticism, rather than on the manifest symptoms of mental health conditions, are also discussed. Keywords:neuroticism, temperament, treatment Neuroticism is typically defined as the tendency to experience frequent and intense negative emotions in response to various sources of stress. While the emotions considered within the pur- view of this trait include the range of negative affect (e.g., fear, irritability, anger, and sadness), the greatest focus has been on the experience of anxious and depressive mood. The perception that the world is a dangerous and threatening place also accompanies this exaggerated negative emotionality, along with beliefs about one’s lack of agency to handle challenging events. Manifestations of this trait may include heightened focus on criticism, either self-generated or from others, as confirming a general sense of inadequacy and perceptions of lack of control over salient events (Barlow, 2002;Barlow, Sauer-Zavala, Carl, Bullis, & Ellard, 2014;Clark & Watson, 2008;Eysenck, 1947;Goldberg, 1993). Barlow and colleagues (Barlow et al., 2014) have forwarded a model to account for the development of neuroticism informed by an interaction of biological and psychological vulnerabilities. To summarize, the general biological risk is evidenced by high heri- tability estimates (40 to 60%) for this trait (e.g.,Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001;Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994;Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003), with genetically mediated neuroticism linked to the neurobiological tendency for heightened reactivity in emotion-generating structures, including amygdala hyperexcitabil- ity, and reduced inhibitory control by prefrontal systems (Keight- ley et al., 2003;Stein, Simmons, Feinstein, & Paulus, 2007; Westlye, Bjørnebekk, Grydeland, Fjell, & Walhovd, 2011). These neural circuits are also influenced by a general psychological vulnerability that includes stressful, traumatic, or related develop- mental experiences during childhood, cultivating the perceptionthat life events are unpredictable and uncontrollable (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007;Lanius, Frewen, Vermetten, & Yehuda, 2010; Rosen & Schulkin, 1998). Taken together, these general biological and psychological vulnerabilities build upon each other to produce the neurotic phenotype. More important, these interacting vulner- abilities were originally described as part of an etiological model for trait anxiety and emotional disorders more generally (Barlow, 2000,2002), suggesting that a neurotic temperament may be a necessary component for the development of a range of psycho- logical conditions. In fact, all the temporal covariance among the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders(DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) constructs of depression and several anxiety disorders can be accounted for by neuroticism (Brown, 2007). Neuroticism has been associated with a wide range of public health problems (see:Lahey, 2009). For example, this trait strongly predicts a variety of mental disorders as well as comor- bidity among them (Clark et al., 1994;Khan, Jacobson, Gardner, Prescott, & Kendler, 2005;Krueger & Markon, 2006;Weinstock & Whisman, 2006), including personality disorders (Henriques- Calado, Duarte-Silva, Junqueira, Sacoto, & Keong, 2014;Sauer- Zavala & Barlow, 2014;Widiger, Verheul, & van den Brink, 1999). Neuroticism has also been associated with a range of physical problems including cardiovascular disease, eczema, asthma, and irritable bowel syndrome (Brickman, Yount, Blaney, Rothberg, & De-Nour, 1996;Smith & MacKenzie, 2006;Suls & Bunde, 2005). Additionally, this trait predicts treatment seeking and response to treatment for both mental disorders and general health concerns (Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor, & Deary, 2007). Considering these public health consequences, it is not surprising that the economic cost of this trait exceeds the cost of common mental disorders (Cuijpers et al., 2010). A discussion of the public health costs associated with neurot- icism prompts consideration of how this trait may be addressed. As noted above, the transactional relationship between genetic and environmental inputs for neuroticism suggests that the inherited contributions of this trait may serve as a predisposition, but not a Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Julianne G. Wilner, and David H. Barlow, De- partment of Psychological and Brain Science, Boston University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Center for Anxiety and Related Disorder, Department of Psychological and Brain Science, Boston University, 648 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215. E-mail:[email protected] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment© 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 8, No. 3, 191–1981949-2715/17/$12.00http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000224 191 mandate. This opens the question of whether neuroticism can be treated directly, rather than separately targeting each manifestation of this trait in the form of separate mental health and physical disorders. This approach is consistent with National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative that challenges researchers to look beyond diagnoses to identify core processes implicated in the development and maintenance of symptoms across a range of disorders (Insel et al., 2010). Targeting neuroticism itself may represent a more efficient and cost-effective means of addressing the wide swath of public health problems associated with it. Malleability of Neuroticism Although personality traits have long been considered stable and inflexible across time (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), there is increasing evidence that neuroticism may be more mal- leable than originally believed. For example, in a review of the literature regarding the stability of personality disorders,Clark (2009)notes that the traits associated with these diagnoses do change slowly over time, with the greatest shifts occurring in the behavioral manifestations of these traits. Further, longitudinal studies of the general population also show gradual age-related decreases in neuroticism and related constructs (i.e., internalizing) that continue into old age (Eaton, Krueger, & Oltmanns, 2011; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008;Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). Overall, this pattern of results suggests that, on average, neuroticism decreases across time; however, when change in neu- roticism has been examined at the individual level using growth modeling (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003), there appears to be great variability in the extent of change, with some people maintaining a stable level of this trait and others changing considerably (Hel- son, Jones, & Kwan, 2002;Small, Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon, 2003). Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that individuals with higher initial levels of neuroticism tend to show less change in this dimension over time, and conversely, individuals with lower initial levels of neuroticism tend to evidence greater change (Brown, 2007). Change in neuroticism has also been explored in the context of individuals seeking treatment forDSMdisorders. For example, in a study that tracked 41 individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) across 8 months (most of whom received some kind of treatment during this time), neuroticism remained stable, despite changes in clinical state (Eaton et al., 2011). Specifically, neurot- icism displayed the same high level of temporal stability in indi- viduals who no longer met criteria for MDD as it did in the subgroup of participants who were depressed at both assessment points. In contrast, others have indeed found changes in neuroti- cism as a function of time and treatment. For example,Brown and colleagues (2007;Brown & Barlow, 2009) examined the temporal course of neuroticism and its relationship to theDSM–IVdisorder constructs of depression, social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder in a large sample (N 606) of outpatients with these disorders. Participants were assessed at intake and then reassessed at 1-year and 2-year follow-ups with the majority (76%) of patients receiving some kind of treatment (of varying quality and duration) during the follow-up period. As expected,DSM–IV disorder constructs improved significantly over time and other temperamental variables (e.g., extraversion) remained stable; neu-roticism, however, evidenced the greatest amount of temporal change and was the dimension associated with the largest treat- ment effect. In addition to studies exploring the malleability of neuroticism in treatment-seeking samples where the treatment received was unspecified, changes in neuroticism have also been examined in the context of specific interventions. First,Kring, Persons, and Thomas (2007)assessed the degree to which neuroticism (de- scribed as negative affectivity in this study) and symptoms of depression and anxiety changed across a course of cognitive– behavioral therapy. Results suggest that neuroticism, as well as symptoms of depression and anxiety, decreased following the 12-week course of treatment; unfortunately, it is difficult to deter- mine whether change in temperament is driving change in symp- toms or vice versa as the study did not include multiple repeated measures of these variables. Additionally, in a large randomized- controlled trial of cognitive therapy (CT) compared with placebo for adults with major depressive disorder (Tang et al., 2009), CT produced greater changes in both neuroticism and extraversion than placebo. However, contrary toBrown’s (2007)results, after controlling for changes in depressive symptoms, the effect of CT on these temperamental variables remained significant only for extraversion. Finally, levels of neuroticism did not appear to change significantly after a course of treatment with dialectical behavior therapy for individuals with borderline personality dis- order (Davenport, Bore, & Campbell, 2010). The inconsistent results described above may be because of the fact that the measurement of neuroticism consists of some combi- nation of stable temperamental variance and variability attributable to generalized distress that is apt to covary with temporal fluctu- ations in the severity of disorders. While this measurement issue, known as mood state distortion, is one possible explanation of the diverse findings with regard to temperamental malleability, there is also a considerable body of compelling research to suggest that these fluctuations represent actual changes in the level of neurot- icism secondary to situational stressors. Given that neuroticism is chiefly defined as the tendency to experience negative emotions, it is reasonable to consider mood state fluctuations that affect self- reported levels of neuroticism as direct expressions of this trait (Costa, Bagby, Herbst, & McCrae, 2005;Widiger & Smith, 2008). Indeed, results from a recent study indicate that measures of neuroticism primarily capture true temperamental variance even in individuals with emotional disorders (Naragon-Gainey, Gallagher, & Brown, 2013). Similarly, to the extent that high neuroticism is necessary for the development of a range of common mental disorders, one would expect that this trait would vary in accordance with the onset or remission of symptoms. In fact, a number of longitudinal studies have controlled for the periodic occurrence of anxious or depres- sive symptoms and still found neuroticism to act independently in predicting anxiety and mood (Lahey, 2009;Spijker, de Graaf, Oldehinkel, Nolen, & Ormel, 2007). Further, while neuroticism predicts the courseDSMdisorders, with higher levels of this trait reflecting less change in symptoms across time, the converse does not appear to occur; that is, initial levels ofDSMdisorders do not predict changes in temperament over time (Brown & Rosellini, 2011;Gershuny & Sher, 1998;Kasch, Rottenberg, Arnow, & Gotlib, 2002; Meyer, Johnson, & Winters, 2001). This pattern of This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 192 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW results, personality traits predicting symptoms but not vice versa, has also been found for personality disorders (Warner et al., 2004). In summary, there appears to be evidence that neuroticism is subject to change slowly over across the course of the life span (e.g.,Clark, 2009;Roberts & Mroczek, 2008); however, the degree to which this and other temperamental variables are malleable in response to treatment remains unclear. The studies described above examined changes in neuroticism in the context of natural- istic treatments or treatments targeting disorder-specific symptoms and have yielded mixed results. Indeed, the research reviewed raises questions about the mechanisms through which neuroticism changes and whether directly targeting this trait in treatment would lead to more definitive results. Treatment of Neuroticism Most studies that have examined changes in temperament in response to psychological treatment, including the studies de- scribed in the previous section, utilize interventions that were not specifically designed to target neuroticism itself, but rather to addressDSMdisorder symptoms. It is possible that the lack of a priori specification regarding how and why these treatments im- pact temperament may have led to the mixed findings on the responsiveness of neuroticism to treatment; in other words, some interventions may be more suited to address neuroticism than others. Emerging research, however, suggests that when neuroti- cism is addressed directly with interventions explicitly designed to target it, change on this construct is more robust. This literature, including specific interventions for neuroticism and the putative pathways through which they target it, are reviewed below. Most of the studies that examine interventions specifically de- signed to target temperament have come from the psychopharma- cology literature (for review, see:Ilieva, 2015;Soskin, Carl, Alp- ert, & Fava, 2012). Serotonergic drug agents (i.e., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors) appear to produce dampening effects on neuroticism (Fu et al., 2004;Harmer, Mackay, Reid, Cowen, & Goodwin, 2006;Harmer et al., 2009;Murphy, Yiend, Lester, Cowen, & Harmer, 2009;Quilty, Meusel, & Bagby, 2008), while catecholaminergic (i.e., noradrenergic/dopaminergic) drugs en- hance extraversion (McCabe, Mishor, Cowen, & Harmer, 2010; Tomarken, Dichter, Freid, Addington, & Shelton, 2004). Clear hypotheses relating these agents to neurobiological changes have been forwarded that underscore their effect on temperament. For example, serotonergic agents likely produce dampening of neurot- icism because of their ability to decrease hyperreactivity of the amygdala in response to fear-inducing stimuli and to inhibit do- paminergic neurotransmission in areas of the prefrontal cortex. In contrast, catecholaminergic agents may be associated with in- creased extraversion because of their ability upregulate noradren- ergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission, particularly within the mesolimbic reward circuitry (Soskin et al., 2012). The fact that specific pharmacological agents display preferential effects on dimensions of temperament suggests that there may be utility in designing behavioral treatments to selectively address tempera- mental variables. Currently, there are few behavioral interventions that are explic- itly designed to target temperament. One of the first examples of a treatment developed to address temperamental vulnerabilities, rather than psychological symptoms, isRapee, Kennedy, Ingram,Edwards, and Sweeney (2005)intervention for behavioral- inhibited children; the goal of addressing behavioral inhibition was to prevent the onset of future anxiety disorders. The program focuses on parenting training to minimize augmentation of the child’s biological nature through environmental interactions and includes psychoeducation about the nature of anxiety, traditional cognitive– behavioral strategies (i.e., exposure and cognitive re- structuring) directed toward personal concerns, and training in behavior management techniques that prevent an overprotective parenting style. Results from randomized controlled trials (Rapee et al., 2005;Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2010) suggest that this program is indeed successful at preventing anx- iety disorders. Although a brief version of this program did not appear to produce significant changes in behavioral inhibition measured via parent report or laboratory observation (Rapee et al., 2010), a more intensive version with higher risk children indeed led to significantly greater reductions in this trait compared with those who did not receive the treatment (Kennedy, Rapee, & Edwards, 2009). Differences among groups increased over time, leadingRapee et al. (2010)to speculate that more intensive inter- ventions directed at temperament might produce an increasing trajectory of change in temperament compared with addressing more surface-level disorder symptoms. The Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emo- tional Disorders (UP;Barlow et al., 2011) represents another example of an intervention explicitly developed to address tem- peramental vulnerabilities, in this case neuroticism, associated with common mental health conditions. As noted above, neuroti- cism is implicated in the development of the range of anxiety, depressive, and related disorders (e.g., somatic symptom disorders, trauma-related disorders), also known as “emotional disorders” (Barlow, 2002) given the role of strong emotions in their devel- opment. In addition to frequently occurring negative emotions, emotional disorders are also characterized by aversive reactions to emotional experiences, leading to problematic efforts to escape or avoid them (see:Barlow et al., 2014;Sauer-Zavala & Barlow, 2014). To address neuroticism, the UP consists of six core treat- ment modules aimed at extinguishing distress in response to the experience of strong emotions. The rationale for this aim is that with fewer aversive reactions to negative emotions when they occur, reliance on avoidant emotion regulation strategies that ex- acerbate symptoms is reduced, which in turn leads to less frequent and intense negative emotions over time. This approach has shown efficacy for the range of anxiety and unipolar depressive disorders (Barlow et al., 2016;Boswell, Anderson, & Barlow, 2014;Ellard, Fairholme, Boisseau, Farchione, & Barlow, 2010;Farchione et al., 2012) and there is preliminary support for the use of the UP with bipolar disorder (Ellard, Deckersbach, Sylvia, Nierenberg, & Bar- low, 2012) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD;Gallagher, 2015). In addition, the emotional disorders framework has been ex- tended BPD and the related phenomena of suicidal and nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior (Bentley, Nock, & Barlow, 2014;Bentley et al., 2016;Sauer-Zavala & Barlow, 2014). The presentation of BPD is largely accounted for by neuroticism, with 4 out of 7 traits (emotional lability, anxiousness, separation insecurity, and depres- sivity) included in the inDSM–5’s alternative model of BPD stemming from the parent factor of neuroticism from the Five Factor Model (FFM) of personality (American Psychiatric Asso- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 193 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM ciation, 2013). Additionally, the impulsive behaviors that charac- terize BPD, including suicidal and nonsuicidal self-injury, are often conceptualized as maladaptive attempts to reduce or escape from intense negative affect (Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006). Although potentially more life-threatening, these BPD-related be- haviors serve a similar function (relief from negative emotions) as leaving a social situation might for an anxiety patient or taking a nap for a depressed patient. Consequently, skills from the UP targeting the aversive, avoidant reactions to emotions that maintain both neuroticism and symptoms of emotional disorders are also relevant for BPD. In fact, the UP or its skill modules have been used to successfully treat BPD (Sauer-Zavala, Bentley, & Wilner, 2016), nonsuicidal self-injury (Bentley, Nock, Sauer-Zavala, Gor- man, & Barlow, 2016), and suicidal thoughts and behaviors (Bent- ley et al., 2016). The modules of the UP have been described in detail elsewhere (e.g.,Payne, Ellard, Farchione, Fairholme, & Barlow, 2014); how- ever, a summary of how the six core modules cultivate a more accepting stance toward emotional experiences is described here. First, by providing information about the adaptive, functional nature of emotions in the psychoeducation module (core module 1), patients begin to cultivate the stance that emotions provide useful information and should not be avoided. Next, patients receive instruction onhowto engage willingly, versus with avoid- ance, with their emotional experiences via mindfulness training (core module 2); specifically, patients are taught the benefits of a present-focused, nonjudgmental attitude toward their emotions through three complimentary experiential exercises. Following the presentation of the UP orientation (e.g., cultivat- ing a more accepting stance toward emotions), patients are taught specific skills that map onto three interacting components of an emotional experience (i.e., thoughts, behaviors, and physical sen- sations). First, patients are encouraged to be more flexible in the way they appraise emotion-eliciting situations (core module 3); patients are instructed to question the automaticity of interpreta- tions, rather than to change maladaptive cognitions—in line with the UPs emphasis on acceptance. The following module, counter- ing emotional behaviors (core module 4), involves the identifica- tion of patient-specific avoidance behaviors that hinder full expo- sure to strong emotions. Patients are then instructed to act counter to their emotion-driven behavioral urges by engaging in activities that may put them in contact with strong emotions in the short- term. Next, patients cultivate a greater tolerance of physical sen- sations through interoceptive exercises (e.g., hyperventilation, breathing through a thin straw) that deliberately provoke the phys- iological feelings associated with strong emotions (core module 5). Finally, treatment culminates with emotion exposures (core mod- ule 6) in which patients engage in a series a series of activities that elicit strong or uncomfortable emotions. Through this practice, patients’ aversive reactions to emotions are gradually extinguished via new learning that emotions are temporary and can be tolerated. There is evidence to suggest that the approach to targeting temper- ament utilized in the UP indeed leads decreases in aversive reactions to emotions that in turn leads to changes in negative affectivity (Sauer-Zavala et al., 2012). Additionally, in the context of a small randomized-controlled trial (i.e.,Farchione et al., 2012), the UP produced small to moderate effects on measures of neuroticism from pre- to posttreatment compared with a waitlist group, and these changes were maintained at the 6-month follow-up assessment point(Carl, Gallagher, Sauer-Zavala, Bentley, & Barlow, 2014). In addi- tion, these changes in neuroticism predicted decreased anxiety and depressive symptoms and reductions in functional impairment. Over- all, the results of this investigation suggest in a preliminary manner that neuroticism can be successfully targeted in treatment and that reductions in this temperamental vulnerability also affect treatment outcomes. More recently,Armstrong and Rimes (2016)conducted a pilot study examining the effect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT;Segal, Teasdale, & Williams, 2002) that had been explicitly modified to directly target levels of neuroticism. The version of MBCT used in this study included references to neuroticism-related constructs, rather than depression-related themes. For example, Ses- sion 1 covered stress-reactivity by introducing the fight-or-flight re- sponse, the role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and un- helpful ways of responding to stress (i.e., overthinking and avoiding). Session 2 involved discussion about the relationship between thoughts and feelings as well as common interpretation biases, and in Session 3, genetic and environmental contributions that make an individual susceptible to stress are discussed. In Session 4 patients learn about the long- and short-term consequences of avoiding stress-eliciting situations and Session 5 extends this discussion by presenting the notion that avoiding such situations leads to increased suffering. Sessions 6 and 7 cover additional maladaptive, neurotic responses to stress including overthinking and self-criticism. Finally, Session 8 ties together the skills for “stress management” that had been learned previously in the treatment. After 8-weeks of treatment, participants in the MBCT condition demonstrated significantly greater reductions in neuroticism than participants in the Internet-based self-help control condition. Conclusions Contrary to theoretical conceptions of personality, research sug- gests that neuroticism may be malleable over time or in response to treatment; however, such findings have been mixed, indicating that additional work is necessary to identify the specific conditions that impact temperament. Interventions that were explicitly developed to target neuroticism appear to have the most consistent effects on this trait. Rapee and colleagues’ (Rapee et al., 2005,2010) work with behaviorally inhibited children addresses the neuroticism by targeting the environmental factors (e.g., parenting styles) that contribute to the general psychological vulnerability (e.g., the belief that the world is a dangerous, uncontrollable place;Barlow et al., 2014) necessary for this trait’s development. In adults, both the UP and neuroticism- focused MCBT address neuroticism by targeting the aversive, avoidant reactions to emotions that, paradoxically, increase the fre- quency and intensity of future negative emotions (Armstrong & Rimes, 2016;Carl et al., 2014). In sum, these efforts provide prelim- inary support for the notion that psychological interventions can indeed address temperamental vulnerabilities, and that such improve- ments are associated with a range of beneficial treatment outcomes (Carl et al., 2014;Farchione et al., 2012;Kennedy et al., 2009). There are several advantages to shifting the focus of treatment to core temperamental vulnerabilities, rather than focusing disorder- specific symptoms. First, the rates of comorbidity among common mental disorders, including emotional disorders and personality disorders, are quite high (Grant et al., 2008;Zanarini et al., 1998). Rather than prioritizing treatment of one condition over another, an This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 194 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW intervention focused on a shared vulnerability (e.g., neuroticism) could lead to simultaneous improvement across symptoms of all co-occurring disorders. Additionally, separate treatments for each Diagnostic and Statistical Manual diagnosis places a burden on practitioners to receive training to competently provide these in- terventions; targeting common temperamental mechanisms may reduce the number of treatments, increasingly the likelihood that empirically supported treatments will be more widely dissemi- nated. Although there are there are theoretical advantages to address- ing neuroticism directly in treatment, there are practical implica- tions associated with moving away from the field’s disorder- specific focus. For example, it is customary, and often necessary, to provide aDSMdiagnosis (rather than levels of neuroticism) for psychotherapy to be reimbursed by insurance companies. This issue is addressable as targeting disorder symptoms and the core temperamental factors that maintain them are not mutually exclu- sive; the goal of addressing neuroticism in treatmentisremission ofDSMdisorders. There may be benefit, however, for insurance companies to shift focus from presence or absence of a particular disorder to levels dimensional variables, like personality traits, as the latter may be more informative with regard to an individual’s functioning (Widiger & Trull, 2007). Additional research is nec- essary to establish clinically significant thresholds for neuroticism that would point to whether treatment should be applied or if meaningful enough gains have been to warrant ending treatment. Additionally, it is important to note that all three behavioral treatments developed to target neuroticism consist of multiple components. Given that they have each been tested only in their entirety, it is unclear whether each component contributes equally to the observed reductions in neuroticism. Future research should explore the unique contributions of each skill included in these treatment packages to identify the components that lead to the most robust and efficient changes in temperament. In addition, future research must also explore the possibility of intervention before a neurotic temperament fully develops. In fact, there is evidence to suggest that behavioral inhibition can be detected as early as 4 months old from crying and salivary cortisol in response to novel stimuli (Moehler et al., 2008). The animal literature provides insight into strategies that may be useful in preventing neuroticism; for example, exposing newborn rats to novel envi- ronments for 3 min a day resulted in less behavioral inhibition (defined as time spent in exploration after weaning) compared with newborn rats that stayed in their home cage (Tang, Reeb- Sutherland, Romeo, & McEwen, 2012). As such, it is important to identify early biological and behavioral markers that may indicate a vulnerability for the development of a neurotic temperament and that could serve as triggers for intervention. In summary, treatments developed to directly target neuroticism indeed appear to impact this trait. However, to our knowledge, only three behavioral trials have been published with the aim of impacting temperamental factors, despite the advantages conferred by targeting shared vulnerabilities instead of disorder symptoms. Future research should not only continue to develop treatments for temperament but also explore which components included in these treatments for neuroticism are the most successful at reducing this trait, as well as how early interventions can occur. References American Psychiatric Association. (2013).Diagnostic and statistical man- ual of mental disorders(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Armstrong, L., & Rimes, K. A. (2016). Mindfulness-based cognitive ther- apy for neuroticism (stress vulnerability): A pilot randomized study. Behavior Therapy, 47,287–298.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015 .12.005 Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory.American Psychologist, 55, 1247–1263.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1247 Barlow, D. H. (2002).Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Fairholme, C., Farchione, T. J., Boisseau, C., Allen, L., & Ehrenreich-May, J. (2011).Unified protocol for the trans- diagnostic treatment of emotional disorders. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Bullis, J. R., Gallagher, M. W., Latin, H., Sauer-Zavala, S.,…Cassiello-Robbins, C. (2016).Equivalence eval- uation of the Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emo- tional Disorders compared to diagnosis-specific CBT for anxiety disor- ders. Manuscript in preparation. Barlow, D. H., Sauer-Zavala, S., Carl, J. R., Bullis, J. R., & Ellard, K. K. (2014). The nature, assessment, and treatment of neuroticism: Back to the Future?Clinical Psychological Science, 2,344 –365.http://dx.doi .org/10.1177/2167702613505532 Bentley, K. H., Nock, M. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). The four-function model of non-suicidal self-injury: Key directions for future research. Clinical Psychological Science, 2,638 – 656.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 2167702613514563 Bentley, K. H., Nock, M. K., Sauer-Zavala, S., & Gorman, B. S. (2016). Examining the effects of two transdiagnostic, emotion-focused interven- tions on non-suicidal self-injury using single-case experimental design. Manuscript submitted for publication. Bentley, K. H., Sauer-Zavala, S., Cassiello-Robbins, C., Conklin, L. R., Vento, S. & Homer, D. (2016).Treating suicidal thoughts and behaviors within an emotional disorders framework: Acceptability and feasibility of the unified protocol in an inpatient setting. Manuscript submitted for publication. Boswell, J. F., Anderson, L. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). An idiographic analysis of change processes in the unified transdiagnostic treatment of depression.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82,1060 – 1071.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037403 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality.Behavior Genetics, 31,243–273.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/ A:1012294324713 Brickman, A. L., Yount, S. E., Blaney, N. T., Rothberg, S. T., & De-Nour, A. K. (1996). Personality traits and long-term health status. The influ- ence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on renal deterioration in type-1 diabetes.Psychosomatics, 37,459 – 468.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/S0033-3182(96)71534-7 Brown, T. A. (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among dimensions of temperament andDSM–IVanxiety and mood disorder constructs.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116,313–328.http://dx .doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.313 Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of theDSM–IVanx- iety and mood disorders: Implications for assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 21,256 –271.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ a0016608 Brown, T. A., & Rosellini, A. J. (2011). The direct and interactive effects of neuroticism and life stress on the severity and longitudinal course of depressive symptoms.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120,844 – 856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023035 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 195 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM Carl, J. R., Gallagher, M. W., Sauer-Zavala, S. E., Bentley, K. H., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). A preliminary investigation of the effects of the unified protocol on temperament.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55,1426 – 1434.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.04.015 Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44,371–394.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat .2005.03.005 Clark, L. (2009). Stability and change in personality disorder.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18,27–31.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01600.x Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (2008). Temperament: An organizing paradigm for trait psychology. In J. P. Oliver, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.) Handbook of personality: Theory and research(3rd ed., pp. 265–286). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,103–116.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.103 Costa, P. T. Jr., Bagby, R. M., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae, R. R. (2005). Personality self-reports are concurrently reliable and valid during acute depressive episodes.Journal of Affective Disorders, 89,45–55.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.010 Cuijpers, P., Smit, F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., de Graaf, R., ten Have, M., & Beekman, A. T. F. (2010). Economic costs of neuroticism: A population- based study.Archives of General Psychiatry, 67,1086 –1093.http://dx .doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.130 Davenport, J., Bore, M., & Campbell, J. (2010). Changes in personality in pre- and post-dialectical behaviour therapy borderline personality disor- der groups: A question of self-control.Australian Psychologist, 45, 59 – 66.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00050060903280512 Eaton, N. R., Krueger, R. F., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2011). Aging and the structure and long-term stability of the internalizing spectrum of per- sonality and psychopathology.Psychology and Aging, 26,987–993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024406 Ellard, K. K., Deckersbach, T., Sylvia, L. G., Nierenberg, A. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2012). Transdiagnostic treatment of bipolar disorder and comor- bid anxiety with the unified protocol: A clinical replication series. Behavior Modification, 36,482–508.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0145445512451272 Ellard, K. K., Fairholme, C. P., Boisseau, C. L., Farchione, T. J., & Barlow, D. H. (2010). Unified protocol for the transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: Protocol development and initial outcome data. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17,88 –101.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/j.cbpra.2009.06.002 Eysenck, J. J. (1947).Dimensions of personality. Oxford, England: Kegan Paul. Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L., Thompson-Hollands, J., Carl, J. R.,…Barlow, D. H. (2012). Unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: A ran- domized controlled trial.Behavior Therapy, 43,666 – 678.http://dx.doi .org/10.1016/j.beth.2012.01.001 Fu, C. H., Williams, S. C., Cleare, A. J., Brammer, M. J., Walsh, N. D., Kim, J.,…Bullmore, E. T. (2004). Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: A prospec- tive, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Ar- chives of General Psychiatry, 61,877– 889.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.61.9.877 Gallagher, M. W. (2015).Unified protocol for posttraumatic stress disor- der. Manuscript submitted for publication. Gershuny, B. S., & Sher, K. J. (1998). The relation between personality and anxiety: Findings from a 3-year prospective study.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107,252–262.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.2 .252Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48,26 –34.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003- 066X.48.1.26 Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stinson, F. S., Saha, T.D.,…Ruan, W. J. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity ofDSM–IVborderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions.The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69,533–545.http://dx .doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v69n0404 Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development.Annual Review of Psychology, 58,145–173.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 Harmer, C. J., Mackay, C. E., Reid, C. B., Cowen, P. J., & Goodwin, G. M. (2006). Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues.Biological Psychiatry, 59,816 – 820.http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015 Harmer, C. J., O’Sullivan, U., Favaron, E., Massey-Chase, R., Ayres, R., Reinecke, A.,…Cowen, P. J. (2009). Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166,1178 –1184.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020149 Helson, R., Jones, C., & Kwan, V. S. (2002). Personality change over 40 years of adulthood: Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of two longi- tudinal samples.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 752–766.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.752 Henriques-Calado, J., Duarte-Silva, M. E., Junqueira, D., Sacoto, C., & Keong, A. M. (2014). Five-factor model personality domains in the prediction of Axis II personality disorders: An exploratory study in late adulthood women non-clinical sample.Personality and Mental Health, 8,115–127.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1250 Ilieva, I. (2015). Enhancement of healthy personality through psychiatric medication: The influence of SSRIs on neuroticism and extraversion. Neuroethics, 8,127–137.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12152-014-9226-z Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., . . . Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167,748 –751.http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp .2010.09091379 Kasch, K. L., Rottenberg, J., Arnow, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Behavioral activation and inhibition systems and the severity and course of depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111,589 –597.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.589 Keightley, M. L., Seminowicz, D. A., Bagby, R. M., Costa, P. T., Fossati, P., & Mayberg, H. S. (2003). Personality influences limbic-cortical interactions during sad mood induction.NeuroImage, 20,2031–2039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.022 Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychi- atric and substance use disorders in men and women.Archives of General Psychiatry, 60,929 –937.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60 .9.929 Kennedy, S. J., Rapee, R. M., & Edwards, S. L. (2009). A selective intervention program for inhibited preschool-aged children of parents with an anxiety disorder: Effects on current anxiety disorders and temperament.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48,602– 609.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181 9f6fa9 Khan, A. A., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric dis- orders.The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186,190 –196.http://dx.doi .org/10.1192/bjp.186.3.190 Kring, A. M., Persons, J. B., & Thomas, C. (2007). Changes in affect during treatment for depression and anxiety.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,1753–1764.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.02.001 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 196 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathol- ogy.Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2,111–133.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213 Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism.American Psychologist, 64,241–256.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015309 Lanius, R. A., Frewen, P. A., Vermetten, E., & Yehuda, R. (2010). Fear conditioning and early life vulnerabilities: Two distinct pathways of emotional dysregulation and brain dysfunction in PTSD.European Journal of Psychotraumatology. Advance online publication.http://dx .doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v1i0.5467 McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2010). Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment.Biological Psychiatry, 67,439 – 445.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001 Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2001). Responsiveness to threat and incentive in Bipolar Disorder: Relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms.Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 133–143.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010929402770 Moehler, E., Kagan, J., Oelkers-Ax, R., Brunner, R., Poustka, L., Haffner, J., & Resch, F. (2008). Infant predictors of behavioural inhibition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26,145–150.http://dx .doi.org/10.1348/026151007X206767 Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A., III. (2003). Modeling intraindividual change in personality traits: Findings from the normative aging study.The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58,p153–p165.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.3.P153 Murphy, S. E., Yiend, J., Lester, K. J., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2009). Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volun- teers.International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 12,169 – 179.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1461145708009164 Naragon-Gainey, K., Gallagher, M. W., & Brown, T. A. (2013). Stable “trait” variance of temperament as a predictor of the temporal course of depression and social phobia.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 611– 623.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032997 Payne, L. A., Ellard, K. K., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). Emotional disorders: A unified transdiagnostic protocol. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.),Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual(5th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Quilty, L. C., Meusel, L. A., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Neuroticism as a mediator of treatment response to SSRIs in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 111,67–73.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j .jad.2008.02.006 Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S., Ingram, M., Edwards, S., & Sweeney, L. (2005). Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhib- ited preschool children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,488 – 497.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488 Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S. J., Ingram, M., Edwards, S. L., & Sweeney, L. (2010). Altering the trajectory of anxiety in at-risk young children.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167,1518 –1525.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1176/appi.ajp.2010.09111619 Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adult- hood.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,31–35.http://dx .doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta- analysis of longitudinal studies.Psychological Bulletin, 132,1–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1 Rosen, J. B., & Schulkin, J. (1998). From normal fear to pathological anxiety.Psychological Review, 105,325–350.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0033-295X.105.2.325 Sauer-Zavala, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). The case for borderline person- ality disorder as an emotional disorder: Implications for Treatment.Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 21,118 –138.http://dx.doi .org/10.1111/cpsp.12063 Sauer-Zavala, S., Bentley, K. H., & Wilner, J. G. (2016). Transdiagnostic treatment of borderline personality disorder and comorbid disorders: A clinical replication series.Journal of Personality Disorders, 30,35–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2015_29_179 Sauer-Zavala, S., Boswell, J. F., Gallagher, M. W., Bentley, K. H., Ametaj, A., & Barlow, D. H. (2012). The role of negative affectivity and negative reactivity to emotions in predicting outcomes in the unified protocol for the transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50,551– 557.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.05.005 Segal, Z., Teasdale, J., & Williams, M. (2002).Mindfulness-Based cogni- tive therapy for depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Shipley, B. A., Weiss, A., Der, G., Taylor, M. D., & Deary, I. J. (2007). Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-year prospective cohort study.Psychosomatic Medicine, 69,923–931.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815abf83 Small, B. J., Hertzog, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Stability and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study.The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psycholog- ical Sciences and Social Sciences, 58,P166 –P176.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1093/geronb/58.3.P166 Smith, T. W., & MacKenzie, J. (2006). Personality and risk of physical illness.Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2,435– 467.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095257 Soskin, D. P., Carl, J. R., Alpert, J., & Fava, M. (2012). Antidepressant effects on emotional temperament: Toward a biobehavioral research paradigm for major depressive disorder.CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 18,441– 451.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-5949.2012.00318.x Spijker, J., de Graaf, R., Oldehinkel, A. J., Nolen, W. A., & Ormel, J. (2007). Are the vulnerability effects of personality and psychosocial functioning on depression accounted for by subthreshold symptoms?Depression and Anx- iety, 24,472– 478.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20252 Stein, M. B., Simmons, A. N., Feinstein, J. S., & Paulus, M. P. (2007). Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,318 – 327.http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.318 Suls, J., & Bunde, J. (2005). Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: The problems and implications of overlap- ping affective dispositions.Psychological Bulletin, 131,260 –300.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.260 Tang, A. C., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., Romeo, R. D., & McEwen, B. S. (2012). Reducing behavioral inhibition to novelty via systematic neo- natal novelty exposure: The influence of maternal hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal regulation.Biological Psychiatry, 72,150 –156.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.021 Tang, T. Z., DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Amsterdam, J., Shelton, R., & Schalet, B. (2009). Personality change during depression treatment: A placebo-controlled trial.Archives of General Psychiatry, 66,1322–1330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.166 Tomarken, A. J., Dichter, G. S., Freid, C., Addington, S., & Shelton, R. C. (2004). Assessing the effects of bupropion SR on mood dimensions of depression.Journal of Affective Disorders, 78,235–241.http://dx.doi .org/10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00306-3 Warner, M. B., Morey, L. C., Finch, J. F., Gunderson, J. G., Skodol, A. E., Sanislow, C. A.,…Grilo, C. M. (2004). The longitudinal relationship of personality traits and disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113,217–227.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.217 Weinstock, L. M., & Whisman, M. A. (2006). Neuroticism as a com- mon feature of the depressive and anxiety disorders: A test of the revised integrative hierarchical model in a national sample.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115,68 –74.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0021-843X.115.1.68 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 197 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM Westlye, L. T., Bjørnebekk, A., Grydeland, H., Fjell, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2011). Linking an anxiety-related personality trait to brain white matter microstructure: Diffusion tensor imaging and harm avoidance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68,369 –377.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archgenpsychiatry.2011.24 Widiger, T. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008).Personality and psychopathology. Handbook of personality: Theory and research(3rd ed., pp. 743–769). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model.AmericanPsychologist, 62,71– 83.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62 .2.71 Widiger, T. A., Verheul, R., & van den Brink, W. (1999). Personality and psychopathology. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),Handbook of personality: Theory and research(2nd ed., pp. 347–366). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Dubo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trikha, A., Levin, A., & Reynolds, V. (1998). Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39,296 –302.http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/S0010-440X(98)90038-4 Members of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate more in this process. If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at [email protected]. Please note the following important points: •To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective review. •To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission within the context of existing research. •To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information. Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example, “social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude change” as well. •Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript thoroughly. APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review- manuscript-ce-video.aspx. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 198 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW
can someone write 2 summaries for the 2 articles attached? the summaries need to be no more than 250 words. summaries should include the background, hypotheses, studies, results, and a brief discussi
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this public ation at: https://www .rese archg ate.ne t/public ation/222651430 Effects of neu roticism and workload history on performance Article in Per sonality and Individual Diff erenc es · Januar y 2004 DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00108-9 CITATIONS 102 READS 1,327 3 author s , including: Some of the author s of this public ation are also w orking on these r elated pr ojects: The SWIFT Off spring Study: A prospective cohort of Inf ants of Mother s with GDM Vie w pr oject The Eff ects of Worklo ad History on Human P erformanc e Vie w pr oject Eugenia F uenzalida University of Oklahoma 20 PUBLICA TIONS 393 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE James B Hittner Colle ge of Charlest on 135 PUBLICATIONS 4,970 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All c ontent f ollowing this p age w as uplo aded by Eugenia F uenzalida on 24 Januar y 2018. The user has requested enhanc ement of the do wnloaded file. Effects of neuroticism and workload history on performance Luz-Eugenia Cox-Fuenzalida a,*, Rhonda Swickert b, James B. Hittner b aDepartment of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, 455 W. Lindsey Street, DHT-705, Norman, OK 73019, USA bDepartment of Psychology, College of Charleston, 66 George Street, Charleston, SC 29424, USA Received 25 July 2002; received in revised form 23 December 2002; accepted 7 February 2003 Abstract This study manipulated workload levels to test Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism by examining indivi- duals’ differential responses to the stress of sudden shifts in workload. A sample of 194 participants who had completed an inventory to assess neuroticism were randomly assigned to either a High-to-Low or Low-to-High testing condition. Participants performed an auditory vigilance task while reaction time and correct responses were assessed. Multiple regression analyses were conducted and results indicated that higher levels of neuroticism were associated with decreased reaction time in both conditions. In addition, individuals higher in neuroticism evidenced fewer correct responses in the Low-to-High workload condi- tion. Results of this study are generally consistent with Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism. #2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords:Neuroticism; Workload history; Task performance; Stress The concept of neuroticism has received considerable attention in the stress and coping litera- ture as it is believed to be an important predictor of how people deal with stressful events. Although many theories have been proposed to explain this dimension, one of the most widely cited is Hans Eysenck’s theory.Eysenck (1967)defined neuroticism as a dimension that ranges from emotional stability to instability and he suggested that individual differences in neuroticism are a result of arousal differences that are mediated by the limbic system. The putative function of the limbic system is to regulate autonomic and emotional reactions, in particular, reactions that are cued by physically or psychologically stressful experiences (Eysenck, 1967). Consequently, individuals who are high in neuroticism, compared with those who are low, are believed to evi- dence greater activation of the limbic system when responding to environmental stressors. This 0191-8869/03/$ – see front matter#2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00108-9 Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-405-325-4511; fax: +1-405-325-4737. E-mail address:[email protected] (L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida). differential autonomic arousal level is believed to form the basis for the behavioral differences observed between those scoring high and low in neuroticism. Although behavioral differences across the neuroticism continuum have been examined using a variety of approaches, one important strategy used to test Eysenck’s arousal-based theory involves the examination of performance differences between high and low neurotics using com- puter-based human performance tasks. Indeed, several studies have demonstrated that indivi- duals scoring high in neuroticism (and trait anxiety) typically perform more poorly on a variety of tasks compared with those low in neuroticism (Eysenck, 1983; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985; Spence & Spence, 1966). Typically, the detrimental effects of neuroticism are particularly evident in studies using vigilance and monitoring tasks—tasks that require high levels of attention (for reviews, seeEysenck, 1983; Matthews & Deary, 1998). For example,Newton, Slade, Butler, and Murphy (1992)conducted a study with 123 participants that employed a visual search task requiring participants to scan a random display of letters to determine the presence or absence of a target letter. This study found neuroticism to be significantly related to slower reaction time in the predicted direction, that is, high neurotics evidenced slower reaction times. Although some studies have found an association between neuroticism and improved performance (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992), generally, findings are consistent withNewton et al. (1992)suggesting that neuro- ticism effectively predicts decrements in performance on vigilance tasks (Darke, 1988; Eysenck, 1992, 1997). In addition to predicting simple vigilance and monitoring task performance, studies have also demonstrated that neuroticism influences performance on other, more complex, types of tasks. For instance,Mayer (1977)conducted a study manipulating task difficulty while examining the effect of trait anxiety on easy versus difficult tasks. He concluded that although high trait anxiety facilitated performance on easy tasks (requiring simple, rote problems—e.g. searching for the letter ‘‘a’’), it had a detrimental effect on difficult tasks (e.g. anagram solving). Although the studies cited earlier have established a link between neuroticism and task perfor- mance, to date little is known about the relationship between neuroticism and basic performance dynamicssuch as variability in workload, or changes in task demand. In other words, while pre- vious research seems to be generally consistent with Eysenck’s arousal-based theory of neuroti- cism, most of these performance-based studies lack generalizability because they do not examine parameters associated with real-world work environments, such as changing workload levels. The study of workload history may help to address this problem. The study of workload history typically involves the manipulation of workload levels over time with individuals moving from either high-to-low, or low-to-high, workload levels. Because per- formance is examined in this manner, the study of workload history might be considered more ecologically valid than less dynamic approaches (e.g. fixed workload level studies). Currently, little is known about the effects of neuroticism and workload history on performance, however, there is a body of work that has examined the manner in which workload shifts influence general performance. One consistent finding reported in the workload history literature suggests that there is a significant decrement in performance following a sudden decrease in workload level (Cumming & Croft, 1973; Goldberg & Stewart, 1980; Matthews, 1986). In a recent study, how- ever,Cox-Fuenzalida (2000)found that either a sudden decrease or increase in workload level results in a significant performance decrement. One explanation for the performance decrements following both types of workload shift is that any change in workload conditions might serve as a 448L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 stimulus that induces stress. In fact, it has been widely discussed in stress literature that one essential component of stress is change in environmental conditions (Holmes & Rahe, 1967; Selye, 1956). Therefore, one might argue that the study of workload history creates the necessary conditions for directly studying stress responses and, in so doing, allows for a direct test of neu- roticism theory. Moreover, by manipulating stress in this manner (i.e. by changing workload levels), this approach may represent a more ecologically valid procedure for testing Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism than has been utilized by previous studies. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the association between neuroticism and task performance across two dif- ferent types of changing workload conditions (Low-to-High versus High-to-Low). Given that higher levels of neuroticism are believed to be associated with greater stress-proneness, and that a sudden change in workload constitutes a stressful event, it was hypothesized that neuroticism would be significantly positively associated with performance decrements following either a sud- den increase or decrease in workload demand. 1. Method 1.1. Participants Participants were 194 undergraduate students (103 females, 91 males) from the University of Oklahoma. Participants were randomly selected from a larger pool of 450 lower division psy- chology students who had participated in a pretesting session in which neuroticism was assessed. Following the random selection procedure, participants were contacted by telephone and invited to participate in the study. 1.2. Materials A computer-based version of theBakan Vigilance Task (1959)was utilized. The Bakan is an auditory vigilance task comprised of a sequence of digits presented to participants by way of earphones. Each training and test trial was 3 min in duration. During each trial, participants were instructed to detect odd–even–odd sequences of digits (the signal, e.g. 7–8–3). Participants were directed to press a specified key on the computer keyboard when they detected a signal. A total of 10 signals were presented in each 3-min period among a string of random digits for the high and low workload conditions (225 and 90 digits total, respectively). Workload level was manipulated by changing the speed of digit presentation (i.e. high workload consisted of 1 digit every 0.8 s and low workload consisted of 1 digit every 2 s). In other words, the number of signals was the same for both high and low workload conditions but the total number of digits, and the difficulty level, varied due to digit rate presentation during the trial. Following signal presentation, the time out period for participants’ responses was 4.8 s. An error was recorded if the participant did not respond within this time frame. The Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI, Form A;Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968) was used to assess the dimension of neuroticism. Items on the Neuroticism Scale of the EPI measure an individual’s level of emotional stability versus emotional reactivity. Higher scores indicate greater levels of neuroticism. Reliability estimates for the EPI neuroticism scale range from 0.84 to 0.92, L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456449 while internal consistency coefficients range from 0.89 to 0.95. Construct validity for this test is reported in the EPI Manual (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968). In the current study the mean of neu- roticism was 11.08 with a standard deviation of 4.51 which is generally consistent with normative data reported in the EPI. 1.3. Procedure When participants arrived at the laboratory they were seated at individual workstations and asked to complete informed consent forms. Workstation partitions minimized distraction from other participants yet permitted individuals to view the experimenter during instruction admin- istration. 1.4. Training and baseline data collection Participants performed the Bakan Vigilance Task during three phases: training, baseline, and the experimental testing session.Table 1presents the counterbalanced orderings for all training, baseline, and test trials. Each of the 194 participants was block randomized to one of the four training/baseline/testing sequences inTable 1. Each participant was first familiarized with the location and operation of the computer response keys relevant for performing the monitoring task. Participants then completed an initial feedback trial followed by an 18-min training session (three, 3-min trials at high difficulty and three, 3-min trials at low difficulty, counterbalanced in their order of presentation) to ensure understanding of task instructions and to lessen the likelihood of the test trials being con- taminated by learning effects. The training regimen was patterned after that used bySchlegel and Gilliland (1990), who found that thorough instructions followed by five to six, 3-min trials were sufficient to produce reasonably asymptotic performance on a variety of human performance tasks. Training session trials were followed by an 18-min baseline session (three, 3-min trials at high difficulty and three, 3-min trials at low difficulty, in counterbalanced order) to establish baseline data for later comparisons. To minimize fatigue, 5-min rest breaks were given between each series of three training and three baseline trials. During these breaks, participants were required to engage in a low-demand distracter task (i.e. completion of participant demographic survey). The three trials of either training or baseline were each 3 min long and were presented with no discernable break between trials (i.e. the task appeared to be nine continuous minutes). Following the second series of three baseline trials and prior to the testing session, participants were given a 15-min break. 1.5. Testing session A participant’s assignment to the High-to-Low or Low-to-High testing condition was based on the training/baseline/testing protocol sequence to which the participant was originally block randomly assigned (seeTable 1). During the test session, participants in the High-to-Low condi- tion engaged in three, 3-min trials at high task difficulty followed immediately by a 3-min trial at low task difficulty. In contrast, participants in the Low-to-High test condition engaged in three, 3- min trials at low task difficulty followed immediately by a 3-min trial at high task difficulty. These 450L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 Table 1 Counterbalanced orderings for training, baseline and test trials for Low-to-High and High-to-Low conditions L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456451 two test conditions created a situation where participants developed a workload history at one workload level and then moved immediately to a dramatically different workload level. Thus, transitions between workload levels during the High-to-Low and Low-to-High test sessions were uninterrupted by rest periods and consequently were perceived by participants as 12 min of con- tinuous work. Participants were tested between the hours of 9.00 a.m. and 4.00 p.m. to control for time-of-day effects (Revelle, Humphreys, Simon, & Gilliland, 1980). 2. Results The two dependent variables, reaction time and number of correct responses, were analyzed separately. In addition, for each dependent variable, the Low-to-High and High-to-Low work- load training conditions were first examined independently before being compared. Finally, for each dependent variable, only the last 3-min trial of the 12-min testing trial was considered in the analyses. Analyses were limited to this trial because only the last trial reflects the effects of chan- ging workload level. Means and standard deviations for the reaction time and correct response variables, for both the baseline and testing conditions, are presented inTable 2. Prior to addressing the primary hypotheses, the univariate distributions for the two dependent variables, across both workload training conditions, and the independent variable of neuroticism, were examined for departures from normality. Due to the significant negative skewness of the correct response variables in both conditions, and the significant positive skewness of the reaction time data in both conditions, these variables were transformed toward normality usingManly’s (1976)single parameter exponential transformation. These transformed variables, in turn, were used in all inferential data analyses. Given that all of the variables in the study were continuously scaled, a multiple regression approach was used to analyze the data. In particular, prior to examining the influence of neuro- ticism on testing performance, the baseline level of performance was controlled for by regressing the testing variable on the baseline variable, and then saving the standardized residuals, or residualized change scores, as a new variable. The residualized change scores—free from the influence of baseline training performance—were then analyzed as the dependent variable of interest. To screen for bivariate outliers, scatterplots were examined (xaxis=neuroticism;y axis=residualized scores) and all extreme outliers that deviated from the swarm of data points were identified. Next, a regression analysis was conducted, excluding the bivariate outliers, in which the residualized scores were regressed onto neuroticism. Results for the reaction time con- ditions are as follows. For the Low-to-High condition (n=89 after removing 2 bivariate outliers), Table 2 Means and standard deviations by testing condition Low-to-High reaction timeHigh-to-Low reaction timeLow-to-High correct responseHigh-to-Low correct response Baseline 0.81 (S.D.=0.37) 0.82 (S.D.=0.15) 6.80 (S.D.=1.97) 9.23 (S.D.=1.54) Testing 0.94 (S.D.=0.45) 0.87 (S.D.=0.24) 6.15 (S.D.=2.08) 8.78 (S.D.=1.70) Note. Reaction time is measured in seconds and correct response scores can range from 0 to 10. 452L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 neuroticism was a significant predictor of testing performance accounting for 5.7% of the var- iance [t(87)=2.29,P=0.024, =0.24]. In the High-to-Low condition (n=103, no bivariate out- liers), neuroticism also significantly predicted testing performance accounting for 4.2% of the variance [t(101)=2.10,P=0.038, =0.21]. To determine whether the strength of association between neuroticism and reaction time performance differed across the two workload conditions (Low-to-High versus High-to-Low), a Fisher’sz-test for independent regression coefficients was conducted. The result of thez-test was statistically nonsignificant (z=0.15,P>0.05). In analyzing the correct response variables, the same approach as described above for the reaction time data was utilized. Regarding the Low-to-High condition (n=84 after removing 7 bivariate outliers), neuroticism was a significant predictor of testing performance accounting for 6.6% of the variance [t(82)= 2.42,P=0.018, = 0.26]. However, in the High-to-Low condi- tion (n=96 after removing 7 bivariate outliers), neuroticism was not a significant predictor of testing performance [t(94)= 1.14,P=0.259, = 0.12]. Despite this difference across the two workload conditions, the result of a Fisher’sz-test comparing the two regression coefficients was not statistically significant (z=0.80,P>0.05). 3. Discussion This study is the first to suggest that the examination of workload history might provide a systematic method for testing Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism. It was hypothesized that higher levels of neuroticism would be associated with significant decrements in performance following changes in workload history. Results revealed significant findings confirming this prediction for reaction time in both conditions (High-to-Low and Low-to-High). Indeed, it appears that at least in terms of reaction time, either a sudden increase or decrease in workload can result in a sig- nificant performance decrement for those scoring higher in neuroticism. Regarding the correct response data, individuals scoring higher in neuroticism showed a significant performance decre- ment only in the Low-to-High workload condition. There was no significant association between neuroticism and performance in the High-to-Low condition, although the data did show a trend in the expected direction. These findings add to a growing body of research suggesting that individuals high in neuroti- cism adapt poorly to changing environmental conditions. For instance, researchers have noted that individuals higher in neuroticism report greater levels of psychological distress when coping with job relocation (Moyle & Parkes, 1999), and they adapt more poorly to shift work changes (Akerstedt and Theorell, 1976). Additionally, it has also been found that individuals higher in neuroticism are less likely to change work environments compared to those lower in neuroticism, perhaps because of the aversiveness of changing work environments (for a review, seeTokar, Fischer, & Subich, 1998). The findings of the present investigation complement the aforemen- tioned studies in that unlike previous research that has focused on psychological and physi- ological outcome measures, the present inquiry documented the interactive effects of neuroticism and changing workload conditions on behavioral performance indices. While the findings of this study complement previous research and appear supportive of Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism, it should be noted that other factors might also account for these effects. For example, one alternative interpretation of these results is that a vigilance L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456453 decrement due to either fatigue or time-on-task, might explain the reduction in performance. However, it is well-documented in the human performance literature that the vigilance decrement is especially sensitive to event rate (Davies & Parasuraman, 1982; Parasuraman & Davies, 1977; See, Howe, Warm & Dember, 1995). That is, according to previous research, an increase in event presentation (e.g. the Low-to High shift) should reduce perceptual sen- sitivity, resulting in degraded performance (Matthews, Davies, Westerman, & Stammers, 2000). Therefore, if time-on-task or fatigue were significant factors in this experiment, a stronger decrement in performance would be expected moving from a Low-to-High than a High-to-Low workload level due to the difference in event rate between the two conditions. Contrary to this prediction, in the present study the performance decrement moving from base- line to testing was generally consistent across the two workload conditions (High-to-Low and Low-to-High) for both reaction time and correct responses (seeTable 2). Consequently, this alternative interpretation based on a vigilance decrement due to time-on-task or fatigue does not appear applicable. Another interpretation of the present findings can be derived from research on cognitive appraisal mechanisms. This literature suggests that an individual’s appraisal of a situation can strongly influence the subjective experience of stress (Lazarus, 1980; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In addition, studies in personality research suggest that neuroticism may be associated with maladaptive appraisal styles. While some individuals are able to handle significant disturbances in stride, others may be anxious when faced with relatively minor challenges (for a review of appraisal and emotion, seeWells & Matthews, 1994). Therefore, individuals who score high in neuroticism, compared with those who score low, might interpret the shift (or change) in work- load more negatively. Such a negative appraisal might adversely affect mood, attention and con- centration, which could then negatively impact performance. Future studies might include a subjective stress questionnaire to evaluate how stressful the sudden change in workload is per- ceived by participants. A final alternative explanation for these results draws upon the relationship between neuroti- cism and coping. To illustrate, a change or shift in workload requires a coping response. The lit- erature suggests that in response to stressful events, individuals scoring higher in neuroticism cope less adaptively (e.g., are less task-focused and more emotion-focused) than those scoring lower in neuroticism (Dorn & Matthews, 1992; Endler & Parker, 1990; Matthews et al., 2000; McCrae & Costa, 1986). In fact, participants high in neuroticism often lack confidence in their abilities, and tend to employ coping strategies (e.g. worry) that are likely to impair performance (Wells & Matthews, 1994). In other words, in response to the sudden workload shift, individuals high in neuroticism might use less adaptive coping strategies thereby resulting in poorer perfor- mance. Future studies could employ a measure to investigate differential coping responses to the workload shifts. In conclusion, results of this study suggest that individuals across the neuroticism continuum respond differently to sudden changes in workload level, though more studies are necessary before any firm conclusions can be drawn. While these findings are consistent with Eysenck’s theoretical model of neuroticism, they might also be accounted for by cognitive-behavioral pro- cesses (e.g. cognitive appraisal, coping strategies). Additional work in this area might begin by utilizing a workload history approach to examine these alternative explanations for the effects of neuroticism on performance. 454L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 Acknowledgements The authors express their gratitude to Dr. Gerald Matthews and Dr. Kirby Gilliland for their many helpful suggestions. References Akerstedt, T., & Theorell, T. (1976). Exposure to night work: serum gastrin reactions, psychosomatic complaints and personality variables.Journal of Psychosomatic Research,20, 479–484. Bakan, P. (1959). Extraversion-introversion and improvement in an auditory vigilance task.British Journal of Psychology,50, 325–332. Cox-Fuenzalida, L. E. (2000).Effect of workload history on vigilance performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, Norman. Cumming, R. W., & Croft, P. G. (1973). Human information processing under varying task demand.Ergonomics,16, 581–586. Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity.Cognition and Emotion,2, 145–154. Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982).The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press. Dorn, L., & Matthews, G. (1992). Two further studies of personality correlates of driver stress.Personality and Individual Differences,13, 949–952. Endler, N., & Parker, J. (1990). Multi-dimensional assessment of coping: a critical review.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58, 844–854. Eysenck, H. J. (1967).The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. (1985).Personality and individual differences. New York: Plenum Press. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968).The Eysenck Personality Inventory manual. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Services. Eysenck, M. W. (1983). Anxiety. In G. R. J. Hockey (Ed.),Stress and fatigue in human performance(pp. 273–295). Chichester, England: Wiley. Eysenck, M. W. (1992).Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. Eysenck, M. W. (1997).Anxiety and cognition: a unified theory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory.Cognition and Emotion,6, 409–434. Goldberg, D. R., & Stewart, M. R. (1980). Memory overload or expectancy effect? ‘Hysteresis’ revisited.Ergonomics, 23, 1173–1178. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale.Journal of Psychosomatic Research,11, 213–218. Lazarus, R. S. (1980). The stress and coping paradigm. In C. Eisdorfer, D. Cohen, & A. Kleinman (Eds.),Conceptual models for psychopathology(pp. 177–214). New York: Spectrum. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984).Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Manly, B. F. J. (1976). Exponential data transformations.The Statistician,25, 37–42. Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J., & Stammers, R. B. (2000).Human performance: cognition, stress and individual differences. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press (part of the Taylor & Francis Group). Matthews, G., & Deary, I. J. (1998).Personality traits. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Matthews, M. L. (1986). The influence of visual workload history on visual performance.Human Factors,28, 623–632. Mayer, R. E. (1977). Problem-solving performance with task overload: effects of self-pacing and trait anxiety.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,9, 283–286. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult sample.Journal of Personality,54, 385–405. Moyle, P., & Parkes, K. (1999). The effects of transitional stress: a relocation study.Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 625–646.L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456455 Newton, T., Slade, P., Butler, N. M., & Murphy, P. (1992). Personality and performance on a simple visual search task. Personality and Individual Differences,13, 381–382. Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1977). A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates(pp. 559–574). New York: Plenum Press. Revelle, W., Humphreys, M., Simon, L., & Gilliland, K. (1980). The interactive effect of personality, time of day, and caffeine: a test of the arousal model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,109, 1–26. Schlegel, R. E., & Gilliland, K. (1990).Evaluation of the Criterion Task Set-Part I CTS Performance and SWAT Data- Baseline Conditions (U) (Tech. Report AAMRL-TR-90-007). Ohio: Wright Patterson. See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S., & Dember, W. N. (1995). Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin,117, 230–249. Selye, H. (1956).The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill. Spence, J. T., & Spence, K. W. (1966). The motivational components of manifest anxiety: drive and drive stimuli. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.),Anxiety and behavior(pp. 361–398). New York: Academic Press. Tokar, D. M., Fischer, A. R., & Subich, L. M. (1998). Personality and vocational behavior: a selective review of the literature, 1993–1997.Journal of Vocational Behavior,53, 115–153. Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994).Attention and emotion. Hove, United Kingdom: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 456L.-E. Cox-Fuenzalida et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 447–456 View publication stats

We’ve proficient writers who can handle both short and long papers, be they academic or non-academic papers, on topics ranging from soup to nuts (both literally and as the saying goes, if you know what we mean). We know how much you care about your grades and academic success. That's why we ensure the highest quality for your assignment. We're ready to help you even in the most critical situation. We're the perfect solution for all your writing needs.
Get a 15% discount on your order using the following coupon code SAVE15
Order a Similar Paper Order a Different Paper